Studies in the psychology of sex, volume 2 by Havelock Ellis. - HTML preview

PLEASE NOTE: This is an HTML preview only and some elements such as links or page numbers may be incorrect.
Download the book in PDF, ePub, Kindle for a complete version.

_Jahrbuch für sexuelle Zwischenstufen_, vol. viii, 1906, p. 124 et seq.

[206] Similarly Numa Praetorius asserts (_Jahrbuch für sexuelle

Zwischenstufen_, vol. viii, p. 732) that even the most virile homosexual

men exhibit feminine traits, and adds that we could scarcely expect it to

be otherwise when we find how constantly homosexual women show masculine

traits.

[207] Näcke, "Die Diagnose der Homosexualität,"

_Neurologisches

Centralblatt_, April 16, 1908.

[208] So also among American boarding-school girls. Thus Margaret Otis

(_Journal of Abnormal Psychology_, June, 1913) has described the

attraction which negro girls exert on white girls at school. The

correspondence of these lovers, and sometimes their method of sex

gratification, may occasionally be of an even coarsely passionate nature.

[209] See "Sexual Selection in Man," vol. iv of these _Studies_.

[210] Hirschfeld (_Die Homosexualität_, p. 283) found that 55 per cent. of

inverts are attracted to qualities unlike their own, and 45 per cent. to

qualities resembling their own, without regard to whether these qualities

belonged to the secondary sexual sphere. It may be added that as regards

the age of the persons they are attracted to, Hirschfeld (p. 281) admits

two main groups, each including about 45 per cent. of the homosexual;

_ephebophils_, attracted to youths between 14 and 21, and _androphils_,

attracted to adults in the prime of life. This division, as may be seen

from the histories included in the present volume, seems to hold good of

British and American inverts.

[211] Hirschfeld, _Die Homosexualität_, ch. v.

[212] Krafft-Ebing tells of an inverted physician (a man of masculine

development and tastes) who had had sexual relations with 600 more or less

inverted men. He observed no tendency to sexual malformation among them,

but very frequently an approximation to a feminine form of body, as well

as insufficient hair, delicate complexion, and high voice. Well-developed

breasts were not rare, and some 10 per cent, showed a taste for feminine

occupations.

[213] A similar condition of gynecomasty has been observed in connection

with inversion by Moll, Laurent, Wey, etc. Olano ("La Secrecion Mamaria en

los Invertidos Sexuales," _Archivos de Criminologia_, May, 1902, p. 305)

further observed a certain amount of mammary secretion in an inverted man,

20 years of age, in Lima.

[214] Hirschfeld finds. 7 per cent, inverts left-handed, and 6 per cent,

partly so. Fliess attaches special importance to left-handedness in

inversion, believing that in left-handed men feminine secondary sexual

characters are marked, and in left-handed women masculine sexual character

(_Der Ablauf des Lebens_, 1906). I am not prepared to deny this statement,

but, more evidence is needed.

[215] This point has been discussed by Hirschfeld, _Die Homosexualität_,

pp. 156-8.

[216] Bloch (_The Sexual Life of Our Time_, p. 500) attaches importance to

this peculiarity, but it must be remembered that a high-pitched voice

occurs frequently in undoubtedly heterosexual men in whom it seems often

associated with high intellectual ability (Havelock Ellis, _A Study of

British Genius_, p. 200).

[217] See, e.g., Hirschfeld, _Die Homosexualität_, p.

151.

[218] On the general signs of these conditions, see, e.g., H. Meige,

"L'Infantilisme, Le Féminisme et les Hermaphrodites Antiques,"

_L'Anthropologie_. 1895; also Hastings Gilford,

"Infantilism," _Lancet_,

February 28 and March 7, 1914.

[219] Merzbach has dealt with the tendency of inverts to adopt special

professions: "Homosexualität und Beruf," _Jahrbuch für sexuelle

Zwischenstufen_, vol. iv, 1902.

[220] Moll's experience in Germany also reveals the prevalence of

inversion among literary men, though, of all occupations, he found the

highest proportion among actors. Jäger has referred to the frequency of

homosexuality among barbers. I have been told that among London

hairdressers homosexuality is so prevalent that there is even a special

attitude which the client may adopt in the chair to make known that he is

an invert. Dr. Kiernan informs me that in Chicago, also, inversion is

specially prevalent among barbers, and he adds that he is acquainted with

two cases among women-barbers, a relatively large proportion. It is not

difficult to understand this, bearing in mind the close physical

association between the barber and his client. "W.G. was a barber's

assistant," writes one of my subjects, "and I took an immense fancy to him

at first-sight. He used to lather me, and the touch of his fingers was a

delight. Later on he shaved me and I always looked forward to going to the

barber's. If he were not able to attend to me I felt an incredible sinking

of heart. The whole day seemed dull and useless. I used to make a mark in

my pocket-diary every time he shaved me."

[221] See, e.g., "Vom Weibmann auf der Bühne," _Jahrbuch für sexuelle

Zwischenstufen_, vol. iii, 1901, p. 313. It is curious to find a

medico-legal record of this connection long before inversion was

recognized. In June, 1833 (see _Annual Register_ under this date), a man

died who had lived as a kept woman under the name of Eliza Edwards. He was

very effeminate in appearance, with beautiful hair, in ringlets two feet

long, and a cracked voice; he played female parts in the theater, "in the

first line of tragedy," and "appeared as a most lady-like woman." The

coroner's jury "strongly recommended to the proper authorities that some

means may be adopted in the disposal of the body which will mark the

ignominy of the crime."

[222] A. Schmid, "Zur Homosexualität," _Zentralblatt für Psychoanalyse_,

vol. i, 1913, p. 237.

[223] See for a summary of various statistics in several countries,

Havelock Ellis, _Man and Woman_, 5th ed., 1914, p. 174; also ib., "The

Psychology of Red," _Popular Science Monthly_, August and September, 1900.

[224] The proportion is not so large, however, as Hirschfeld (_Die

Homosexualität_, p. 314) now finds in Germany, where inverts are better

informed on the subject of this anomaly, for here 95 per cent. regard

their feelings as natural.

CHAPTER VI.

THE THEORY OF SEXUAL INVERSION.

What is Sexual Inversion?--Causes of Diverging Views--

The Theory of

Suggestion Unworkable--Importance of the Congenital Element in

Inversion--The Freudian Theory--Embryonic Hermaphroditism as a Key to

Inversion--Inversion as a Variation or "Sport"--

Comparison with

Color-blindness, Color-hearing, and Similar Abnormalities--What is an

Abnormality?--Not Necessarily a Disease--Relation of Inversion to

Degeneration--Exciting Causes of Inversion--Not Operative in the Absence

of Predisposition.

The analysis of these cases leads directly up to a question of the first

importance: What is sexual inversion? Is it, as many would have us

believe, an abominably acquired vice, to be stamped out by the prison? or

is it, as a few assert, a beneficial variety of human emotion which should

be tolerated or even fostered? Is it a diseased condition which qualifies

its subject for the lunatic asylum? or is it a natural monstrosity, a

human "sport," the manifestations of which must be regulated when they

become antisocial? There is probably an element of truth in more than one

of these views. Very widely divergent views of sexual inversion are

largely justified by the position and attitude of the investigator. It is

natural that the police-official should find that his cases are largely

mere examples of disgusting vice and crime. It is natural that the asylum

superintendent should find that we are chiefly dealing with a form of

insanity. It is equally natural that the sexual invert himself should find

that he and his inverted friends are not so very unlike ordinary persons.

We have to recognize the influence of professional and personal bias and

the influence of environment.

There have been two main streams of tendency in the views regarding sexual

inversion: one seeking to enlarge the sphere of the acquired (represented

by Binet,--who, however, recognized predisposition,--

Schrenck-Notzing, and

recently the Freudians), the other seeking to enlarge the sphere of the

congenital (represented by Krafft-Ebing, Moll, Féré, and today by the

majority of authorities). There is, as usually happens, truth in both

these views. But, inasmuch as those who represent the acquired view often

deny any congenital element, we are called upon to discuss the question.

The view that sexual inversion is entirely explained by the influence of

early association, or of "suggestion," is an attractive one and at first

sight it seems to be supported by what we know of erotic fetichism, by

which a woman's hair, or foot, or even clothing, becomes the focus of a

man's sexual aspirations. But it must be remembered that what we see in

erotic fetichism is merely the exaggeration of a normal impulse; every

lover is to some extent excited by his mistress's hair, or foot, or

clothing. Even here, therefore, there is really what may fairly be

regarded as a congenital element; and, moreover, there is reason to

believe that the erotic fetichist usually displays the further congenital

element of hereditary neurosis. Therefore, the analogy with erotic

fetichism does not bring much help to those who argue that inversion is

purely acquired. It must also be pointed out that the argument for

acquired or suggested inversion logically involves the assertion that

normal sexuality is also acquired or suggested. If a man becomes attracted

to his own sex simply because the fact or the image of such attraction is

brought before him, then we are bound to believe that a man becomes

attracted to the opposite sex only because the fact or the image of such

attraction is brought before him. Such a theory is unworkable. In nearly

every country of the world men associate with men, and women with women;

if association and suggestion were the only influential causes, then

inversion, instead of being the exception, ought to be the rule throughout

the human species, if not, indeed, throughout the whole zoölogical series.

We should, moreover, have to admit that the most fundamental human

instinct is so constituted as to be equally well adapted for sterility as

for that propagation of the race which, as a matter of fact, we find

dominant throughout the whole of life. We must, therefore, put aside

entirely the notion that the direction of the sexual impulse is merely a

suggested phenomenon; such a notion is entirely opposed to observation and

experience, and will with difficulty fit into a rational biological

scheme.

The Freudians--alike of the orthodox and the heterodox schools--have

sometimes contributed, unintentionally or not, to revive the now

antiquated conception of homosexuality as an acquired phenomenon, and that

by insisting that its mechanism is a purely psychic though unconscious

process which may be readjusted to the normal order by psychoanalytic

methods. Freud first put forth a comprehensive statement of his view of

homosexuality in the original and pregnant little book, _Drei Abhandlungen

zur Sexualtheorie_ (1905), and has elsewhere frequently touched on the

subject, as have many other psychoanalysts, including Alfred Adler and

Stekel, who no longer belong to the orthodox Freudian school. When inverts

are psycho-analytically studied, Freud believes, it is found that in early

childhood they go through a phase of intense but brief fixation on a

woman, usually the mother, or perhaps sister. Then, an internal censure

inhibiting this incestuous impulse, they overcome it by identifying

themselves with women and taking refuge in Narcissism, the self becoming

the sexual object. Finally they look for youthful males resembling

themselves, whom they love as their mothers loved them.

Their pursuit of

men is thus determined by their flight from women. This view has been set

forth not only by Freud but by Sadger, Stekel, and many others.[225] Freud

himself, however, is careful to state that this process only represents

one type of stunted sexual activity, and that the problem of inversion is

complex and diversified.

This view may be said to assume a bisexual constitution as

normal, and homosexuality arises by the suppression, owing to

some accident, of the heterosexual component, and the path

through an autoerotic process of Narcissism to homosexuality. On

this general Freudian conception of homosexuality numerous

variations have been based, and separate features specially

emphasized, by individual psychoanalysts. Thus Sadger considers

that, beneath the male individual loved by the invert, a female

is concealed, and that this fact may be revealed by psychoanalysis which removes the upper layer of the psychic

palimpsest; he believes that this disposition of the invert is

favored by a frequent mixture of male and female traits in his

near relatives; originally, "it is not man whom the homosexual

man loves and desires but man and woman together in one form";

the heterosexual element is later suppressed, and then pure

inversion is left. Further, developing Freud's view of the

importance of anal eroticism (Freud, _Sammlung Kleiner Schriften

zur Neurosenlehre_, vol. ii), Sadger thinks that it is even the

rule for a passive invert to have experienced anal eroticism in

childhood and been frequently subjected to enemas, which have led

to the desire for the anal intromission of the penis.

(_Medizinische Klinik_, 1909, No. 2.) Jekels pushes this doctrine

further and declares that all inverts are really passive; the

invert is, in his love, he states, both subject and object; he

identifies himself with his mother and sees in the object of his

love his own youthful person. And what, Jekels asks, is the aim

of this mental arrangement? It can scarcely by other, he replies,

than in the part of the mother to stimulate the anal region of

the object which has now become himself, and to procure the same

pleasure which in childhood he experienced when his mother

satisfied his anal eroticism. Jekels regards this view as the

continuation and concretization of Freud's interpretation; and

the main point in homosexuality, even when apparently passive,

becomes the craving for anal-erotic satisfaction (L.

Jekels,

"Einige Bemerkungen zur Trieblehre," _Internationale Zeitschrift

für Aerztliche Psychoanalyse_, Sept., 1913). Most psychoanalysts

are cautious in denying a constitutional or congenital basis to

inversion, though they leave it in the background.

Ferenczi, in

an interesting attempt to classify the homosexual (_Internationale Zeitschrift für Aerztliche Psychoanalyse_,

March, 1914), remarks: "Psychoanalytic investigation shows that

under the name of homosexuality the most various psychic states

are thrown together, on the one hand true constitutional

anomalies (inversion, or subject homoeroticism), on the other

hand psychoneurotic obsessional conditions (object homoeroticism,

or obsessional homoeroticism). The individual of the first kind

essentially feels himself a woman who wishes to be loved by a

man, while the other represents a neurotic flight from women

rather than sympathy to men." The constitutional basis is very

definitely accepted by Rudolf Ortvay who points out (_Internationale Zeitschrift für Aerztliche Psychoanalyse_, Jan.,

1914) that the biological doctrine of recessives and dominants in

heredity helps to make clear the emergence or suppression of

homosexuality on a bisexual disposition. "Infantile events," he

adds, "which, according to Freud, decide the sexual relations of

adults, can only exert their operation on the foundation of an

organic predisposition, infantile impressions being determined by

hereditary predisposition." Isador Coriat, on the other hand,

while recognizing two forms of inversion, incomplete and

complete, boldly asserts that it is never congenital and never

transmitted through heredity; it is always

"originated through a

definite unconscious mechanism" (Coriat,

"Homosexuality," _New

York Medical Journal_, March 22, 1913). Adler's view of

homosexuality, as of other allied conditions, differs from that

of most psychoanalysts by insisting on the presence of an

original organic defect which the subject seeks to fortify into a

point of strength; he accepts two chief components of inversion:

a vagueness as to sexual differences and a process of

self-assurance in the form of rebellion and defiance, and even

the feminism of the invert may become a method of gaining power

(A. Adler, _Ueber den Neurösen Charakter_, 1912, p.

21).

The mechanism of the genesis of homosexuality put forward by Freud need

not be dismissed offhand. Freud has often manifested the insight of

genius, and he refrains from molding his conceptions in those inflexible

shapes which have sometimes been adopted by the more dogmatic

psychoanalysts who have followed him. Nor need we be unduly shocked by the

"incestuous" air of the "Oedipus Complex,"[226] as it is commonly called,

which figures as a component of the process. The word

"incest," though it

has been used by Freud himself, seems scarcely a proper word to apply to

the vague and elementary feelings of children, especially when those

feelings scarcely pass beyond a stage of non-localized and therefore

really presexual feelings (in the ordinary use of the term "sexual") which

may be regarded as natural and normal. The Freudian conception is

misrepresented and prejudiced by the statement that it involves

"incest."[227] When a child loves its mother with an entire love, that

love necessarily involves the germs which in later life become separated

and developed into sexual love, but it is inaccurate to term this love of

the child "incestuous." It is quite easily conceivable that the psychic

mechanism of the establishment of homosexuality has in some cases

corresponded to the course described by Freud. It may also be admitted

that, as psychoanalysts claim, the pronounced _horror feminæ_ occasionally

found in male inverts may plausibly be regarded as the reversal of an

early and disappointed feminine attraction. But it is impossible to regard

this mechanism as invariable or even frequent. It is quite true, and I

have found ample evidence of the fact, that inverts are often very closely

attached to their mothers, even to a greater degree, indeed, than is the

rule among normal children, and often like to be in constant association

with their mothers. But this attraction is quite misunderstood if it is

regarded as a peculiarly sexual attraction. Indeed, the whole point of the

attraction is that the inverted boy vaguely feels his own feminine

disposition and so shuns the uncongenial amusements and society of his own

sex for the sympathy and community of tastes which he finds concentrated

in his mother. So far from such association being evidence of sexual

attraction it might more reasonably be regarded as evidence of its

absence; just as the association of boys among themselves, and of girls

among themselves, even in co-educational schools, is proof of the

prevalence of heterosexual rather than of homosexual feeling. Confirmation

of this point of view may be found in the fact--

overlooked and sometimes

even denied by psychoanalysts--that frequently, even in early childhood

and simultaneously with this community of feeling with his mother, the

homosexual boy is already experiencing the predominant fascination of the

male. He feels it long before the age at which Narcissism is apt to occur,

or at which self-consciousness has become sufficiently developed to allow

the internal censure on unpermitted emotions to operate, or any flight

from them to take place. Moreover, while most authorities have rarely been

able to find any clear evidence of the sexual attraction of male inverts

in childhood to mother or sister,[228] an attraction of this kind to

father or brother seems less difficult to find, and if found it is

incompatible with the typical Freudian process. In my own observation,

among the Histories here recorded, there are at least two clear examples

of such an attraction in childhood. It must further be said that any

theory of the etiology of homosexuality which leaves out of account the

hereditary factor in inversion cannot be admitted. The evidence for the

frequency of homosexuality among the near relatives of the inverted is now

indisputable. I have traced it in a considerable proportion of cases, and

in many of these the evidence is unquestionable and altogether independent

of the statement of the subject himself, whose opinion may be held to be

possibly biased or unreliable.[229] This hereditary factor seems indeed to

be called for by the Freudian theory itself. On that theory we need to

know how it is that the subject passes through psychic phases, and reaches

an emotional disposition, so unlike that of normal persona. The existence

of a definite hereditary tendency in a homosexual direction removes that

difficulty. Freud himself recognizes this and clearly asserts congenital

psycho-sexual constitution, which must involve predisposition. On a

general survey, therefore, it would appear that, on the psychic side, we

may accept the reality of unconscious dynamic processes which in

particular cases may be of the Freudian or similar type.

But while the

study of such mechanisms may illuminate the psychology of homosexuality,

they leave untouched the fundamental organic factors now accepted by most

authorities.[230]

The rational way of regarding the normal sexual instinct is as an inborn

organic impulse, reaching full development about the time of puberty.[231]

During the period of development suggestion and association may come in to

play a part in defining the object of the emotion; the soil is now ready,

but the variety of seeds likely to thrive in it is limited. That there is

a greater indefiniteness in the aim of the sexual impulse at this period

we may well believe. This is shown not only by occasional tentative signs

of sexual emotion directed toward the same sex in childhood, but by the

frequently ideal and unlocalized character of the normal passion even at

puberty. But the channel of sexual emotion is not thereby turned into an

abnormal path. Whenever this happens we are bound to believe--and we have

many grounds for believing--that we are dealing with an organism which

from the beginning is abnormal. The same seed of suggestion is sown in

various soils; in the many it dies out; in the few it flourishes. The

cause can only be a difference in the soil.

If, then, we must postulate a congenital abnormality in order to account

satisfactorily for at least a large proportion of sexual inverts, wherein

does that abnormality consist? Ulrichs explained the matter by saying that

in sexual inverts a male body coexists with a female soul: _anima

muliebris in corpore virile inclusa_. Even writers of scientific eminence,

like Magnan and Gley, have adopted